定性与定量结果相矛盾时,如何处理?

来源:百度文库 编辑:神马文学网 时间:2024/04/30 12:29:56
定性与定量结果相矛盾时,如何处理? [精华]
kenan

版主
发帖: 259
于2001-12-03 13:39 When quantitative findings seem to contradict qualitative findings, which should you believe? Often, the automatic assumption is that the quantitative findings must be right. After all, the quantitative data come from a large, “scientific” sample and the findings are expressed in a number to one or two decimal places, so they must be precise. Others believe that the qualitative findings must be right. After all, they go into deeper human motivation instead of counting the answers to relatively superficial - or even wrong - questions.
Neither stand is necessarily correct. Qualitative research and quantitative research complement each other. In any given case either may be correct or both may be correct - about somewhat different questions - even though the findings may seem contradictory. Of course, they may both be wrong. So how do you interpret the findings in this kind of situation?
This is a very difficult subject that raises fundamental questions about how we separate truth from illusion. There are no easy answers here. I wish I could say that in a conflict between qualitative and quantitative findings, always believe quantitative or always believe qualitative. That is how some people operate, particularly people who have a quantitative bent. They think that qualitative is fuzzy stuff that you do to refine the questionnaire before you do the “real research.” They would be well reminded that a number is the result of a mathematical operation, not necessarily the solution to a problem.
Also, let’s remember that bad research can yield any findings whatsoever. So one would expect bad research of any kind to contradict bad research of any other kind. I’m assuming that we are talking here about soundly designed research, competently executed.
Let’s take some examples that have happened to me several times.
Example 1: Desired attributes differ between focus groups and survey.
You conduct a series of focus groups and determine that a given set of product attributes are most desired by prospects. A survey produces an entirely different mix of attributes and/or an entirely different order of the attributes. Which should you believe?
As I said, there are no easy answers here. Let me point out a few issues for your consideration as you figure out the apparent discrepancy. Determining which set of attributes most closely fits the actual situation will probably depend on how the questions were asked. Product attributes are a funny thing. When you ask people what they want in a particular product, they tend to come back to you with the “must-haves.” These are the attributes that are absolutely necessary to their even considering the product. However, all products must have these attributes in order to be considered. So they are not what I call the decisive attributes, the attributes upon which people decide when considering their final alternatives. For example, if you ask people why they bought a particular minivan, you will get answers such as quality, service, styling, etc. However, if you ask people to describe their experience of purchasing a minivan, or ask them to tell stories about purchasing a minivan, or use a variety of other projective techniques, you’ll soon discover that minivans are purchased based upon cup holders and other things that most of us would regard as trivial amenities.
The basic attributes having been satisfied, people look for the small points of differentiation. They would never choose cup holders even if someone were astute enough to put that attribute into a survey. They probably wouldn’t even bring it up in a focus group under direct questioning, except in the form of a wisecrack. (Many a true word is said in jest. Take the wisecracks seriously.)
So, the point here is to examine very carefully exactly how the questions were asked and how meaningful the answers are likely to be. When you ask people for attribute lists, or have them rank attribute lists, all you are getting is the answer to the question, “How do people consciously rank attribute lists?” How people actually act on their ranked attributes is an entirely different matter.
To directly answer the question of whether qualitative or quantitative is likely to have yielded better answers: if approached in the traditional ways, in this case neither is likely to be correct. The best ways, in my opinion, to identify which attributes are actually causing brand choice are indirect, projective qualitative techniques and indirect statistical quantitative techniques.
Ponder this classic example of the testing of brightly colored inexpensive cameras: People in focus groups who were shown the cameras loved the idea. People answering surveys were relatively neutral. But when people were allowed to pick one of the cameras to take home, they all picked black! Behavior trumps talk.
Example 2: Focus groups love product, sales prove otherwise.
A series of focus groups tells you that opinion leaders, customers and prospects love the product. But the sales curve is declining, and surveys indicate that while there is no dissatisfaction with the product, people have no intention of buying it.
Example 3: Sales are soaring, surveys indicate high eagerness to try, but focus groups indicate product dissatisfaction.
Conversely, the sales curve of a new product is going through the roof, and surveys indicate that people are extremely eager to try the product. They even indicate that they would pay much more for the product than its current selling price. The situation is interpreted as a smashingly successful product launch, with even additional pent-up demand. The product management team and their agencies are drinking champagne. However, you discover, in some focus groups that were originally designed as a disaster check on some ad copy, that the initial users are encountering difficulties after a few months of product use and dropping the product. In fact, the initial users are extremely disappointed, and many are angry.
Let’s look at Example 2 and 3. First of all, it’s important to understand the nature of sampling. I’m fond of saying that one person’s bias is another person’s sample. When you include early users of a product, you are automatically selecting experts, innovators and early adopters. That is often an extremely productive thing to do, and I wish it were done more often. But remember that you are automatically selecting a different type of person than you will reach in an overall blanket survey. Also, since these are very small numbers of people, they will make up only a very small part of the sales curve. So, when the experts, innovators and early adopters are raving about a product, as in Example 2, you are working with a very promising product indeed. Surveys and sales curves are likely to seriously underestimate the potential of the product - as long as a way can be found to bridge the chasm to the early majority. This product is likely to succeed no matter what the quantitative data suggest.
Example No. 3 is a very frequent occurrence that has cost many product managers their jobs. Sales are soaring, surveys are positive, but focus groups indicate that people are dropping the product after a period of time. For instance, I have worked with about a dozen new drugs over the years where the initial sales curve was extremely positive, as were many other initial quantitative measures. I call this the “try and drop curve.” As long as increasing numbers of people are trying a new product, the sales curve will go up even if most of them are subsequently dropping the product. The main way to tell a try and drop curve from a successful product curve without waiting for the inevitable precipitous drop is to track groups of triers. The most expeditious and timely way to do that is in focus groups. These people may have used the product that day. In telephone groups or online groups they may even be using the product (a snack food or a drink) during the group. When those groups tell you that the product doesn’t work or has other fatal flaws, run for the hills. Or, if it is a really good product, do something to fix the mess. If you don’t act quickly, the word-of- mouth is likely to overwhelm the rest of the marketing.
Example 4: Focus groups love the idea, surveys of early adopters reject it.
People love the product in the qualitative concept development phase. However, surveys among the potential early adopters indicate that the early adopters feel that the product is taking the wrong approach and favor specifically-named other products. Which do you believe?
This is also a hard call, but the product probably is a loser. People can easily get overly enthusiastic or overly negative in concept development groups. You can read more about how to deal with these problems in an article atwww.mnav.com/contest.htm. You have to listen very clearly for respondents’ reasons, attitudes and emotions. For instance, groups of computer store owners loved the Apple Lisa and predicted its success. It was clear that they were reacting to an elegant technological breakthrough but couldn’t answer the inevitable cost-effectiveness questions. “Cool” does not sell a $9,000 computer. So it was obvious that the interpretation (it’s a loser) was the opposite of what they were actually saying (it’s a winner).
Conversely, when the opinion leaders initially hate the product because it lacks technological sophistication, and the more typical people love its simplicity, the money is with the typical people. The Palm Pilot is a great example. So are AOL and Windows.
Example 5: The majority of qualitative respondents say one thing (e.g., prefer Concept X) but a majority of the quantitative respondents differ (e.g., prefer Concept Y instead).
More likely than not, the quantitative finding is correct (unless some special factors like those previously mentioned were at fault) because the small qualitative sample just happened to over-represent the X-lovers by the luck of the draw. This is in fact the reason that quant and qual conflict most often.
Actually, people who say these results conflict are probably making the mistake of thinking that the qualitative serves the quantitative purpose of estimating majority preferences. Rather, an appropriate purpose of the qualitative would have been to discover and understand the thoughts and feelings behind preferences for X vs. Y, whereas an appropriate purpose of the quantitative should have been to estimate the percentages of people who hold particular thoughts, feelings, and preferences regarding the concepts. (This last example and analysis were contributed by Peter DePaulo. Thanks Pete!)
The point of all this is that you have to know what exactly has been asked, of whom, and how the answers fit into the rest of the situation. You will inevitably get different views from different perspectives, but that can round out the picture if the perspectives have been carefully chosen. The meta-point here is that you either need to hire, or need to be or become, a savvy, thoughtful marketing research consultant, not a technician of qual or quant.
I hope that this has given you some things to think about when qualitative and quantitative research show different findings. This article doesn’t even begin to address the complexity of the fundamental differences between qual and quant. That’s going to take a whole book that I urge someone (it’s not going to be me!) to write.
from George Silverman is president of Market Navigation, Inc., an Orangeburg, N.J., research firm. He is a member of the Qualitative Research Consultants Association.
kenan

版主
发帖: 259
于2001-12-05 19:46 没人回复呀,看来这里搞定性的还不多:)其实这篇文章很实用的,里面的例子是我们在实践中经常遇到的,只是我们往往忽视矛盾的存在,或者自己找个理由来解释。
eLan

Null
.
发帖: 1195
于2001-12-05 19:58 可能因为是英文的缘故,大家都懒得看
to err is human
to include error in analysis is statistics
hotriver


发帖: 136
于2001-12-06 14:51 这个帖子的内容相当好,最重要的不是其中一些具体实用的例子,而是它提醒了我们,不能只是看到数据结果,而要理解数据结果的适用性及其隐含的意义。过去在企业里,经常很遗憾地看到决策人员由于缺乏相应的知识而被劣质报告中的武断结论所误导,而更遗憾的是市场研究行业的从业人员也同样缺乏相应的知识而未能给出合理的建议。
另外,我认为在先做定性,后做定量的情况下,例子1不应该出现,否则就应该是方案的设计有问题。
hawkyring


发帖: 13
于2001-12-06 21:26 good article.
as I know, to avoid such problem. They usually do qualitative research before quatitative research.
rabbitchan


发帖: 6
于2004-08-26 17:04 好象在这里提关于定性的很少?本人对定性比较感兴趣,但是也不知道从何下手,希望能多看到你们的经验之谈,感激之无限。
从零开始,什么都模糊。
写意



发帖: 43
来自: 上海
于2004-08-26 18:34 这英文看得累死了,而且作者是中国人吧,语感不舒畅啊。能不能用中文总结下到底讨论什么问题?
JeffreyLee


发帖: 1
于2006-05-29 17:49 这个作者写的英文看起来很辛苦,
大家有没有一些quanti的例子,例如一些问题该怎么问而不该怎么问?
这些都会影响答案的。